Kathleen M. Holehan, MA, BCBA, LBA, and Thomas Zane, PhD, BCBA-D
Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas

Is there science behind sensory diets for autism?As the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) continues to increase, individuals and families continue to require evidence-based interventions that effectively promote adaptive behavior and decrease maladaptive behavior. Unfortunately, some interventions that have not been empirically proven gain popularity and become common practice. This seems to be the case with sensory-based interventions such as sensory diets, which continue to lack empirical support but are popular for individuals with ASD (Barton et al., 2015; Case-Smith & Arbesman, 2014).

What is the historical link between ASD and Sensory Diets?

Patricia Wilbarger proposed the concept of sensory diets in 1995. Specifically, sensory diets are derived from the principles of sensory integration (SI) and sensory processing theories (Wilbarger, 1995). Jean Ayers developed SI theory based on principles within the disciplines of neuroscience, biology, psychology, occupational therapy, and education (1972). That is, SI theory hypothesizes that some individuals with intellectual and developmental disabilities have trouble processing and integrating sensory information (e.g., auditory, vestibular, proprioceptive, tactile, visual), affecting their adaptive and maladaptive repertoires (Ayers, 1972, 1979, 1989). Sensory processing theory was developed by Winnie Dunn (1997) and is based on occupational and educational counseling principles. Specifically, sensory processing theory proposes four sensory processing patterns (i.e., low registration, sensory avoiding, sensory seeking, and sensory sensitivity), which characterize the perceptual process. Dunn proposed these patterns arise from individual differences in neurological thresholds for stimulation and self-regulation (Dunn, 1997, 2001).

Like the developers of SI and sensory processing theories, Wilbarger based sensory diet interventions on the assumption that individuals (with or without ASD) need certain types of sensorimotor experiences (i.e., passive [e.g., weighted vest, massage], active [e.g., jumping, climbing]) to stay alert, stay organized, and to successfully participate in many aspects of life. Wilbarger further proposed that sensory diets can provide controlled sensory input or utilize already existing sensory input for an individual to maintain a “regulated behavioral state” (i.e., exhibit adaptive behaviors). Timing, intensity, duration, and type of sensory stimuli are the key elements of sensory diets and are determined on an individual basis (Wilbarger & Wilbarger, 2002). Specifically, a sensory therapist will provide the individual with a schedule of sensory-based activities (e.g., brushing, joint compression, climbing, weighted vest) to be implemented across all environments (e.g., home, school) throughout the day, aimed to fulfill that individual’s sensory needs (Baranek, 2002). It should be noted that these experiences can require considerable time cumulatively over the course of the week. As sensory diets, sensory integration theory, and sensory processing theory focus on increasing adaptive behavior and functional skills by receiving various types of sensory input, these approaches are most frequently used by occupational therapists as part of an intervention package (Schaaf & Miller, 2005).

Ayers, Dunn, and Wilbarger designed their theories and interventions for individuals diagnosed with or who display sensory processing disorders. A high percent (80-90%) of individuals with ASD are reported to have sensory processing disorders (Schaaf & Miller, 2005). That is, it is alleged that the brain has difficulty receiving and responding to sensory input. The high frequency of individuals diagnosed with ASD who also display behaviors commonly found with sensory processing disorders is not surprising as the behaviors which comprise ASD are similar to those which comprise sensory processing disorders (e.g., maladaptive behavior, poor social participation, aversion to sensory stimuli, excessive seeking of sensory stimuli; Schaaf & Miller, 2005). Because of the high percentage of individuals with ASD who also display behaviors found with a sensory processing disorder, many occupational therapists recommend and use sensory diets with individuals with ASD (Schaaf & Miller, 2005).

Is there any research to support Sensory Diets?

Before reviewing the existing research on sensory diets, it is important to remember that sensory diet interventions were developed for individuals displaying behaviors associated with a diagnosis of sensory processing disorders. However, sensory processing disorders are not recognized by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders V (DSM-V; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In fact, the American Psychiatric Association (APA) Board of Trustees outright rejected the addition of sensory processing disorders to the manual stating there was not enough research to even consider the addition (Grohol, 2018).

Although sensory diets are commonly used with individuals with ASD, research specifically on sensory diets is sparse. One case study conducted by Stagnitti et al. in 1999 used a sensory diet intervention for a 5-year-old boy diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome and sensory defensiveness. Following several direct and indirect assessments (i.e., Miller assessment for preschoolers [Miller, 1988], a sensory checklist [e.g., Schoen, Miller, & Sullivan, 2014], parent interview, observations), a “sensory summation technique” or sensory diet was recommended by an occupational therapist. The sensory diet consisted of a combination of brushing the participant’s arms, back, and legs with a soft scrub brush, performing ten joint compressions to the arms and legs, providing sensory input to the fingers and toes, rubbing with a towel following bathing, and placing soft textures on the bed. Initially, the brushing, joint compressions, and sensory input were to be provided at two-hour intervals during the day for two weeks; however, due to schedule demands, the family implemented the diet three times a day for the first week and four to five times a day the second week. Anecdotal reports from the parents indicated that the participant’s maladaptive behavior improved, he began participating in groups, and was no longer sensory defensive. However, data continued to be collected over the next two months, and over that time span, the participant once again began displaying the maladaptive behavior. The researchers indicated that it was the end of the school term, and they believed “fatigue” was the reason for the resurgence. However, the following school term, five months after the initial assessment, the participant’s maladaptive behavior maintained despite continuing the sensory diet. Further analysis of the results suggests several limitations to the study. First, the sensory diet was not implemented as originally recommended due to the family schedule. Second, no systematic data were collected. Results were based on anecdotal reports by parents. Lastly, there were several confounding interventions introduced with the sensory diet after the initial implementation, which may have been responsible for the decrease in maladaptive behavior and an increase in adaptive behavior.

Although there is not a breadth of research specifically on sensory diets, there is a small amount of research focused on SI interventions. Because sensory diets are derived from SI principles, it is worth briefly examining the literature surrounding this theory and intervention. There have been several systematic reviews and meta-analyses conducted in an attempt to demonstrate the effectiveness of SI interventions (e.g., Baranek, 2002; Case-Smith & Arbesman, 2008; Case-Smith et al., 2014; Hoehn & Baumeister, 1994; May-Benson & Koomar, 2010; Ottenbacher, 1982b; Polatajko et al., 1992; Vargas & Camilli, 1999); however, results have varied considerably.

For example, the Ottenbacher (1982b) meta-analysis concluded that SI interventions demonstrated a positive treatment effect with regard to the motor outcomes of children with disabilities compared to the control group of children who received no intervention. However, the treatment effect found in this study has not been replicated in other meta-analyses. A systematic review conducted by Polatajko et al. (1992) concluded that although the SI intervention was effective, it was no more effective than other interventions in the control group (e.g., perceptual-motor approach, tutoring). A later systematic review by Hoehn & Baumeister (1994) re-analyzed some of the data from the Polatajko et al. review and further confirmed SI was no more effective than other interventions. May-Benson and Koomar (2010) systematically reviewed SI research and found similar results, in that SI interventions are more effective when compared to control groups of no treatment and are equally as effective to other interventions. Further, Hoehn & Baumeister concluded that maturation may be responsible for any hypothesized SI treatment effect. In 2002, Baranek conducted a systematic review of the SI research conducted with children with ASD and concluded that although previous reviews had suggested positive outcomes of SI interventions, all research on this topic is limited by small sample sizes and weak designs (e.g., inadequate control groups, no experimental control). More recently, Case-Smith et al. (2014) conducted a systematic review of SI interventions for children diagnosed with ASD and found that SI interventions such as sensory diets implemented across a child’s day are not effective, as the interventions lack rigor and vary in implementation. However, SI interventions implemented in a clinic with a therapist demonstrated some efficacy. Overall, these meta-analyses and systematic reviews demonstrated that the research and evidence supporting SI interventions is lacking.

Position Statements

Major organizations have made public declarations against sensory diets and sensory procedures. The American Academy of Pediatrics (2012) concluded that not only should a sensory processing disorder not be diagnosed, but that there was limited evidence of effectiveness and that parents and other care providers be apprised of this. The National Clearinghouse on Autism Evidence and Practice (Steinbrenner et al., 2020) recently published their updated (from 2011) review of autism treatments and levels of evidence. It indicated that there has been no empirical study published on the effectiveness of sensory diets since 2011, but that there have been publications suggesting it is not evidenced-based, and so recommends caution when considering the diet.

Future Research

Due to the overall limited literature showing the effectiveness of SI and specifically sensory diets, one must consider whether future research is necessary. This approach has been studied extensively (eight meta-analyses) with equivocal results. It seems very unlikely that future research will find a causal connection between SI procedures and positive client outcomes. If future research is done, more information is needed regarding sensory diets and sensory integration intervention effects, the factors influencing these effects, the maintenance of these effects, and the effectiveness of the interventions compared to others (i.e., instead of comparing to no intervention). Specifically, comparisons to other interventions should be conducted using methods and research designs that have been validated and demonstrate experimental (i.e., single case designs [e.g., reversal design, alternating treatments design]).

What is the bottom line?

Simply put, there have not been a sufficient amount of empirical studies conducted showing effectiveness for consumers and service providers to conclude that SI procedures are effective. The evidence that exists, at best, is mixed, mostly showing ineffectiveness. As scientists and practitioners, we have an ethical responsibility to use evidence-based interventions. Until an empirical consensus is reached regarding the effectiveness of sensory diets and SI interventions, they should not be used.

References

American Academy of Pediatrics (2012). Sensory integration therapies for children with developmental and behavioral disorders. American Academy of Pediatrics. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-0876

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Publisher.

Ayers, A. J. (1972). Sensory integration and learning disorders. Western Psychological Services.

Ayers, A. J. (1979). Sensory integration and the child. Western Psychological Services.

Ayers, A. J. (1989). Sensory integration and praxis tests. Western Psychological Services.

Baranek, G. T. (2002). Efficacy of sensory and motor interventions for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(5), 397-422. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1020541906063

Barton, E. E., Reichow, B., Schnitz, A., Smith, I. C., & Sherlock, D. (2015). A systematic review of sensory-based interventions for children with disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 37, 64-80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.11.006

Case-Smith, J., & Arbesman, M. (2008). Evidence-based review of interventions for autism used in or of relevance to occupational therapy. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 62(4), 416-429. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.62.4.416

Case-Smith, J., Weaver, L. L., & Fristad, M. A. (2014). A systematic review of sensory processing interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 19(2), 133-148. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361313517762

Dunn, W. (1997). The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children and their families: A conceptual model. Infants and Young Children, 9(4), 23-35. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001163-199704000-00005

Dunn, W. (2001). The sensations of everyday life: Empirical, theoretical, and pragmatic considerations. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 55(6), 608-620. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.55.6.608

Grohol, J. M., (2018). Final DSM 5 approved by the American psychiatric association. PsychCentral. https://www.psychiatry.org/psychiatrists/practice/dsm

Hoehn, T. P., & Baumeister, A. A. (1994). A critique of the application of sensory integration therapy to children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(6), 338-350. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221949402700601

May-Benson, T. A., & Koomar, J. A. (2010). Systematic review of the research evidence examining the effectiveness of interventions using a sensory integrative approach for children. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 64(3), 403-414. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2010.09071

Miller, L. (1988). Miller Assessment for Preschoolers. Administration and interpretation. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Ottenbacher, K. (1982b). Sensory integration therapy: Affect or effect. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 36, 571-578.

Polatajko, H. J., Kaplan, B. J., & Wilson, B. N. (1992). Sensory integration treatment for children with learning disabilities: Its status 20 years later. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health, 12(6), 323-341. https://doi.org/10.1177/153944929201200601

Schaaf, R. C., & Miller, L. J. (2005). Occupational therapy using sensory integrative approach for children with developmental disabilities. Mental Retardation and DevelopmentalDisabilities, 11(2), 143-148. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20067

Schoen, S. A., Miller, L. J., & Sullivan, J. C. (2014). Measurement in sensory modulation: The sensory processing scale assessment. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 68(5), 522-530.

Stagnitti, K., Raison, P., & Ryan, P. (1999). Sensory defectiveness syndrome: A pediatric perspective and case study. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 46(4), 175-187. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1630.1999.00197.x

Steinbrenner, J. R., Hume, K., Odom, S. L., Morin, K. L., Nowell, S. W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey, S., McIntyre, N. S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage, M. N. (2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth, and young adults with Autism. The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute, National Clearinghouse on Autism Evidence and Practice Review Team.

Vargas, S,. & Camilli, G. (1999). A meta-analysis of research on sensory integration treatment. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 53, 189-198. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.53.2.189

Wilbarger, P. (1995). The sensory diet: Activity programs based on sensory processing theory. Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Newsletter, 18(2), 1-4.

Wilbarger, J., & Wilbarger, P. (2002). Clinical application of the sensory diet. In A.C. Bundy, S.J. Lane, & E.A. Murray (2nd Ed.). Sensory Integration: Theory and Practice.

Citation for this article:

Holehan, K. M., & Zane, T. (2021). Sensory diets: Is there science behind that? Science in Autism Treatment, 18(1).

Other ASAT Related Articles:

Other ASAT Related Research Synopses:

Related Media Watch Letters:

 

#Centerbased

Print Friendly, PDF & Email